Abbildungen der Seite
PDF
EPUB

the hole H, so as to admit more rays from each luminous point of RB, the images would become brighter, but they. would become at the same time indistinct, as the rays from one point of the object would mix with those from adjacent points, and at the boundaries of the colours R, Y, and B, the one colour would obliterate the other. In order, therefore, to obtain sufficiently bright images of visible objects we must use lenses, which have the property of forming distinct images behind them, at a point called their focus. If we widen the hole H, and place in it a lens whose focus is at y, for an object at the same distance, HY, it will form a bright and distinct image, br, of the same size as the object RB. If we remove the lens, and place another in H, whose focus is at y', for a distance HY, an image, b'r', half of the size of RB, will be formed at that point; and if we substitute for this lens another, whose focus is at y", a distinct image, b′′r", twice the size of the object, will be formed, the size of the image being always to that of the object as their respective distances from the hole or lens at H.

With the aid of these results, which any person may confirm by making the experiments, we shall easily understand how we see external objects by means of the images formed in the eye. The human eye, a section and a front view of which is shewn in Fig. 5, A, is almost a sphere. Its outer membrane, ABCDE, or MNO, Fig. 5, B, consists of a tough substance, and is called the sclerotic coat, which forms the white of the eye, A, seen in the front view. The front part of the eyeball, cxD, which resembles a small watch-glass, is perfectly transparent, and is called the cornea. Behind it is the iris, cabe, or c in the front view, which is

a circular disc, with a hole, ab, in its centre, called the pupil, or black of the eye. It is, as it were, the window of the eye, through which all the light from visible objects must pass.

[merged small][merged small][graphic][merged small][merged small][merged small][subsumed]

The iris has different colours in different persons, black, blue, or grey; and the pupil, ab, or H, has the power of contracting or enlarging its size according as the light which enters it is more or less bright. In sunlight it is very small, and in twilight its size is considerable. Behind the iris, and close to it, is a doubly convex lens, df, or LL in Fig. 5, B, called

D

A

B

M

N

FIG. 5, B.

the crystalline lens. It is more convex or round on the inner side, and it is suspended by the ciliary processes at LC, LC, by which it is supposed to be moved towards and from H, in order to accommodate the eye to different dis

tances, or obtain distinct vision at these distances. At the back of the eye is a thin pulpy transparent membrane, rr o rr, or vvv, called the retina, which, like the ground glass of a camera obscura, receives the images of visible objects. This membrane is an expansion of the optic nerve o, or a in Fig. 5, A, which passes to the brain, and, by a process of which we are ignorant, gives us vision of the objects whose images are formed on its expanded surface. The globular form of the eye is maintained by two fluids which fill it, the aqueous humour, which lies between the crystalline lens and the cornea, and the vitreous humour, zz, which fills the back of the eye.

But though we are ignorant of the manner in which the mind takes cognizance through the brain of the images on the retina, and may probably never know it, we can determine experimentally the laws by which we obtain, through their images on the retina, a knowledge of the direction, the position, and the form of external objects.

If the eye MN consisted only of a hollow ball with a small aperture н, an inverted image, ab, of any external object AB would be formed on the retina ror, exactly as in Fig. 4. A ray of light from A passing through H would strike the retina at a, and one from B would strike the retina at b. If the hole H is very small the inverted image ab would be very distinct, but very obscure. If the hole were the size of the pupil the image would be sufficiently luminous, but very indistinct. To remedy this the crystalline lens is placed behind the pupil, and gives distinctness to the image ab formed in its focus. The image, however, still remains inverted, a ray from the upper part A of the object necessarily falling on the lower part a of the retina,

and a ray from the lower part в of the object upon the upper part 6 of the retina. Now, it has been proved by accurate experiments that in whatever direction a ray АHa falls upon the retina, it gives us the vision of the point A from which it proceeds, or causes us to see that point, in a direction perpendicular to the retina at a, the point on which it falls. It has also been proved that the human eye is nearly spherical, and that a line drawn perpendicular to the retina from any point a of the image ab will very nearly pass through the corresponding point A of the object AB,1 so that the point A is, in virtue of this law, which is called the Law of visible direction, seen in nearly its true direction.

When we look at any object, AB, for example, we see only one point of it distinctly. In Fig. 5 the point D only is seen distinctly, and every point from D to A, and from D to B, less distinctly. The point of distinct vision on the retina is at d, corresponding with the point D of the object which is seen distinctly. This point d is the centre of the retina at the extremity of the line A Ha, called the optical axis of the eye, passing through the centre of the lens Lh, and the centre of the pupil. The point of distinct vision d corresponds with a small hole in the retina called the Foramen centrale, or central hole, from its being in the centre of the membrane. When we wish to see the points A and B, or any other point of the object, we turn the eye upon them, so that their image may fall upon the central point d. This is done so easily and quickly that every point of an object is seen distinctly in an instant, and we obtain the most perfect knowledge of its form, colour, and direction.

1 Edinburgh Transactions, vol. xv. p. 349, 1843; or Philosophical Magazine, vol. xxv. pp. 356, 439, May and June 1844.

Vision

The law of distinct vision may be thus expressed. is most distinct when it is performed by the central point of the retina, and the distinctness decreases with the distance from the central point. It is a curious fact, however, that the most distinct point d is the least sensitive to light, and that the sensitiveness increases with the distance from that point. This is proved by the remarkable fact, that when an astronomer cannot see a very minute star by looking at it directly along the optical axis dD, he can see it by looking away from it, and bringing its image upon a more sensitive part of the retina.

But though we see with one eye the direction in which any object or point of an object is situated, we do not see its position, or the distance from the eye at which it is placed. If a small luminous point or flame is put into a dark room by another person, we cannot with one eye form anything like a correct estimate of its distance. Even in good light we cannot with one eye snuff a candle, or pour wine into a small glass at arm's length. In monocular vision, we learn from experience to estimate all distances, but particularly great ones, by various means, which are called the criteria of distance; but it is only with both eyes that we can estimate with anything like accuracy the distance of objects not far from us.

The criteria of distance, by which we are enabled with one eye to form an approximate estimate of the distance of objects are five in number.

1. The interposition of numerous objects between the eye and the object whose distance we are appreciating. A distance at sea appears much shorter than the same distance on land, marked with houses, trees, and other objects; and

« ZurückWeiter »